bell notificationshomepageloginNewPostedit profiledmBox

Hoots : What causes notes to appear to follow a progression of melodic expectations? How does this work? If I play a single note, it by itself doesn't evoke a melody or tune. But if I play a sequence of notes, suddenly relationships - freshhoot.com

10% popularity   0 Reactions

What causes notes to appear to follow a progression of melodic expectations? How does this work?
If I play a single note, it by itself doesn't evoke a melody or tune. But if I play a sequence of notes, suddenly relationships spring up between them. For instance, if I play CDEFGABC, I don't hear isolated notes but instead a scale. 

Why do these relationships occur? What causes a sequence of notes to seem related?

CLARIFICATION:

This question is about successive notes not simultaneous notes. Answerers please emphasize melody not chords.


Load Full (6)

Login to follow hoots

6 Comments

Sorted by latest first Latest Oldest Best

10% popularity   0 Reactions

A sequence of notes, like a sequence of numbers, can be related, or unrelated. As far as numbers go, the series of numbers 1,2,3,4,5,6,7 etc. have a specific relationship to one another and we can easily recognize this pattern. Likewise the numbers 2,4,6,8 have a certain identifiable relationship as do the numbers 7,14,21,28,35. There can be a sense of order in the relationships - regardless of what order you place the numbers - or a sense of order based on the order of the numbers which form a predictable pattern.

If we construct a string of random numbers such as 3,4, 7, 12,19,22,37,23,72,86,43 - we may not see a relationship or pattern that is identifiable or predictable.

Music is very similar to math in that it deals with sound frequencies that can be measured in terms of a certain number of oscillations or cycles per second. A string vibrating a given number of times per second or millisecond - will create a specific sound through the process of converting the vibrations to sound waves which in turn will translate into sound we hear via corresponding vibrations of the basilar membrane in our inner ear.

A string vibrating at a different rate of speed will result in a different note. If there is a simple mathematical relationship between the two frequencies, we will detect a pattern or sense that the two notes somehow belong together or go together or have an identifiable and detectable relationship to one another.

For example if the ratio between the frequency of two notes is 2 to 1 or two times as fast (two wave peaks for one note to every one wave peak of the other) - this 2:1 frequency is known as an octave. It's the same note - exactly one octave higher (vibrating exactly twice as fast = octave higher or half as fast = octave lower). That is a simple mathematical relationship.

Between a note and the octave (doubling or halving of frequency) are many other possible notes with varying ratios. In Western music, we divide an octave into 7 possible notes using mathematical logic and simple integer ratios.

The relationship between the mathematical characteristics of the soundprint (frequency of crest of sound waves plotted on a graph) of the notes that are in a given key (7 possible notes per key in Western music) all have a mathematically definable relationship to one another with the intervals between each note of the key being dependent on the type key (major, harmonic minor, natural minor, melodic minor etc.).

If not for the equal temperament tuning system (another topic altogether) each major key will contain a note that has a frequency relationship to the tonic note (aka root note - C in the key of C, D in the key of D etc.) of 3:2 which makes a perfect fifth. The perfect fifth is a stable harmonious interval between notes and will be found in most keys - even in equal temperament. A pitch ratio of 4:3 makes a perfect fourth which is another simple interval that sounds stable and pleasing.

The seven divisions of an octave that make up the notes for a given key are derived by application of mathematical principals and formulas. In real life, adjustments have been made with a system of tuning called "12 tone equal temperament" that deviate slightly from the perfect mathematical formulas - to allow for the use of keyboards that have only 88 keys and can transpose music to any key.

For the most part, the resulting pitch ratios determine the notes that form a given key. One way to see how these ratios can be used to construct a key is by application of the perfect fifth (the most stable interval besides the unison 1:1 and octave 2:1) to a series of notes to determine which notes belong in a given key.

For example to determine the notes that will fit in the key of C major you start with the root note C and take the first perfect fifth inversion (work backward 7 steps in descending order) and you arrive at the note of F. Another way to look at this is to determine which note the root note is the perfect fifth of which C is the note that is a perfect fifth above F. A perfect 5th interval in Western music is always the starting note and the note that is exactly 7 semi tones above the starting note (3:2 pitch ratio) So if you start on F and go up a fifth - you land on C. Up a fifth from C gives you G. Up a fifth from G gives you D, up a fifth from D gives you A, up a fifth from A gives you E and up a fifth from E gives you B.

F—C—G—D—A—E—B - each note a perfect fifth higher than the preceding note and the end result of counting 7 steps each time gives you all of the notes contained in the key of C major. The same process works for the key of D, D# E, F, F# G etc.

The point being, there is a mathematical formula that can be applied to determine the notes in a given key. This mathematical formula will insure that the pitch ratios between all the notes of a given key (equal temperament notwithstanding) are related by the fact that they are multiples or divisors of one another and thus the sound waves will eventually coincide (every so many peaks) and blend. Notes not in the key will not have their waves coincide and they will clash - and not seem to go together.

Certain frequencies of sound or vibration patterns of the sound waves, imply other frequencies to our brain that are mathematically divisible by the primary fundamental frequency. So the brain implies or infers certain notes that will go with the fundamental note based on this mathematical inference phenomena that occurs in our auditory processing center. So we can form melodies in our head and the notes we "hear" in our head will naturally be in the same key. The other notes are "implied" by this mathematical processing.

Sound waves that don't overlap or cannot be mathematically derived by a multiple of or divisor of one another, create an uncomfortable sensation to our ears known as beating. The sound waves clash with one another instead of blending together. Notes not in the same key will be detectable as not belonging together by our brain because they won't fit together via the implied inference to one another in the subliminal overtones.

That is why certain notes seem to belong together whether playing a scale which is a derivative of a particular key and spans an octave - or playing a melody within a particular key. The very sophisticated and still not completely understood computer in the auditory processing center of our brains knows which notes belong together because it can do the math subliminally - faster than our calculators.

EDIT: We instinctively know which notes seem to go together. Our brains gravitate towards related notes and melodic patterns naturally. To some extent our musical preferences can develop a cultural bias based on what we regularly hear from childhood on - and the emotions that are associated with particular types of music we are exposed to. But we are born with a certain musical sense that in the absence of cultural influence would evolve as explained by the mathematical relationships between notes described above. Music theory is nothing more than an attempt to logically or scientifically explain why we instinctively know what notes go together - so we can apply said theory to help us create music that is pleasing to our ears.

The first time a cave man (or woman) accidentally stumbled upon a perfect fifth while blowing through hollow sticks of varying lengths, he/she became the worlds first "rock star". The other cave people could not explain why the sequence of notes sounded so pleasing together, because music theory had not yet been developed. But they did know it sounded good.


10% popularity   0 Reactions

You have noticed that the bulk of Western music is tonal. There is a note that feels like home, like you've arrived to a restful place, called tonic. When you are playing a tune that is entirely in the key of C, C is tonic (the first degree of the scale).

Some notes are a bit like homing pigeons -- tending to move toward home (tonic). The note that does this the strongest is the seventh degree of the scale -- in C major, this would be B. This note is called the leading tone because it leads so strongly to tonic.

The fifth degree of the scale also leads to tonic, but not as strongly. The second degree of the scale does too, but this one is a bit more subtle.

If you get interested in learning some music theory, you will learn about different types of cadences. I won't try to explain what a cadence is here, but cadences are a fun topic, very accessible, and will help you with what you are wondering about.


10% popularity   0 Reactions

I'll note that most of the answers so far have assumed that because the question used a diatonic series of notes to pose itself, the questioner wants to know how tonality works, and thus needed basic info on tuning, consonance and dissonance, etc.

I read the question as being about melodic expectation, or, to use the technical terms music theorists use, implications and realizations. The major theorists here have been Leonard Meyer and his pupil Eugene Narmour. I would recommend Meyer's Emotion and Meaning in Music (available for free at rhythmcoglab.coursepress.yale.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/5/2014/10/Emotion-and-Meaning-in-Music.pdf), which treats this question as a general one in perception and cognition.

Meyer uses Gestalt theory and some early cognitive science to create a theory of how various melodic gestures create a pattern in our minds that implies various continuations with different degrees of probability. When the continuation occurs, we feel various musical "feelings" based on how likely we judged that continuation to be. (Of course, all this can happen below the conscious level.) The continuous play of expectation and realization gives music its sense of forward motion.

Meyer identified a small set of "rules" that seemed to govern our perception of what was normal or probable (or "good") in the continuation of a melodic pattern, mostly common-sense ones akin to Newtonian laws:

a melodic line moving in a certain direction raises the expectation that it will continue to move in that direction [cde leads to cdef]
a melodic pattern that creates a gap raises the expectation that the gap will be filled [cdf leads to cdfe]
a melodic line can create a "border" or limit, which will then raise expectation that the limit will be transcended [cd cd cd cd leads to cde]

and so on.

Meyer ramifies his theory by allowing expectations or implications to nest or to overlap, and for the same melodic event to satisfy expectations at multiple levels [cd cd cd cd cdf leads to cde]. Or for implications to clash [cdf# leads to...either cde or cdg, or both, so that cdge would give one feeling, cdeg another, and cdf#g# yet another...]

The power of this type of perceptually-based theorizing is that it can be tested, and it provides a clear model of how abstract melodic relationships can give rise to intersubjectively valid emotional responses to music. (I'm not saying that Meyer defined the precise "meaning" of every melodic implication in words, just that his principle of how emotions arise from music "itself," rather than association we have with it, is very clear.)

It's also tonality independent: a pattern like [cf#bc#] has clear implications in terms of melodic extension and gap filling, even though it does not have obvious tonal implications. A lot of "atonal" music works very obviously this way, setting up polar or limit pitches and then moving past them, using strong directionality or reversals to channel our expectations, etc.


10% popularity   0 Reactions

One reason is habit: when you hear for the very first time a sequence of sounds (either a scale or a chord), it has very poor meaning and evokes very few feelings.
I play guitar and when I learn a new type of chord or scale I "begin appreciating" it after a period of practice: I find a "meaning" for that chord or scale and I can insert it properly inside a song in harmony with the rest, sharing the same meaning with my colleagues. I'm sure that I haven't this ability before the period of practice I mentioned.
The reason why you hear the major diatonic scale in the way you describe is mainly because - I suppose - you're occidental or live in an occidental context. You have heard it many and many times since when you were a little child and music that you hear every day is based mainly upon it and so you can give it an exact meaning, shared with other occidentals you know.
An oriental person who has never heard occidental music gives it poorer meaning than the one you give, whereas you may find "strange" oriental sonority.


10% popularity   0 Reactions

In western music, (one should ALWAYS qualify that!) our familiar diatonic scale (do re mi fa sol la ti do) is based on the harmonic series and on the notes that are small-integer-ratios to the "tonic."

The tonic note (do) is the note of the diatonic scale that is most at rest. Practically the whole of our western perception of melody is based on motion away from the tonic, or increasing the "tension," and then releasing or resolving that tension by moving back towards the tonic. The note that we perceive as tonic is set up by the peculiar pattern of whole steps and half-steps between notes...those again defined not arbitrarily but as the close-neighbor-ratios.

The whole of our perception of "tonality" is centered around this concept of tension being created, then resolved.


10% popularity   0 Reactions

I'm not sure if this is relevant, but have a read anyway.

I believe that it was the Pythagoreans of ancient Greece (circa 6th Century BCE) who first formally studied the relationships between sounds, and specifically why some notes sound more pleasing together than others. They did this in the context of a vibrating string. You ask :

Why do these relationships occur? What causes a sequence of notes to seem related?

and you comment :

I was hoping someone had studied this scientifically and evidence could be provided to explain it better.

What the Pythagoreans discovered was that there is a relationship between the "fundamental" frequency of a vibrating string (or a blown pipe, etc.) and its overtones. Specifically that the overtones relate to the fundamental tone according to simple integer ratios. This implied that notes sound best together when their frequencies exist in simple integer rations.

For example, if one plucks a string of a given length together with a string of half that length, then the resulting pitches sound pleasing together. The smaller integer ratios (1:2, 1:3, 1:4, 2:3, 3:4, etc.) produce the most pleasing combinations.

In western music, the choice of twelve tones in the octave and the tempering of our musical instruments are the result of these observations. The identification of our scales coincide with those tones whose ratios are the simpler integer values.

The Pythagorean's work was completed and formalized in the 17th century by Mersenne, who formulated Mersenne's Laws.


Back to top Use Dark theme